The Role of Mental Schemes in the Formation of Traditional Maragheh Housing Pattern

Document Type : Scientific Research

Authors

1 Tabriz Islamic art university

2 MSc in Islamic Architecture, Architecture and Urbanism Faculty, Tabriz Islamic Art University, Tabriz, Iran

Abstract

Research Problem: In recent decades, Iranian architecture has experienced a break with its environmental, historical and cultural context. This shift toward mainstream trends in the contemporary world has brought the human aspect of Iranian architecture to its most materialistic embodiment. As a result, architecture has been reduced to its minimal essence as shelter, subsequently, affecting the inhabitants’ mental schemas of residential environments.
Research Question: This article seeks to answer the following questions: What mental schemas have helped shape the traditional housing pattern of Maragheh city? And which of the schemas have had the highest impact on shaping this pattern?
Research Method: The relationship between mental schemas and their external representation (that is, the housing pattern) can be examined through cognitive processes that are rooted in cultural factors. After a review of relevant literature, this paper investigates the role of mental schemas in shaping place patterns. To this end, we identified the different types of behavioral schemas through cognitive processes (perception, cognition, evaluation, emotion, behavior). Then, in the second step, we investigated these schemas through field surveys in the form of questionnaires and interviews with residents of traditional houses, and examined their relationship with traditional housing patterns in Maragheh. Traditional houses in Maragheh can be categorized into four patterns. After analyzing field data, we identified the most effective schemas in the formation of traditional housing in Maragheh.
The Most Important Results and Conclusion: Our findings indicate that the following schemas have had the highest impact on the formation of the housing pattern in Maragheh in order of arrangement: Context, Emotion, Role, Fact and Concept, Strategy/Procedure and Self/Person. The Context schema with its subcomponents of privacy, nature, hospitality, and personal space received the highest mean score (3.32) as the first factor. The Emotion schema with its subcomponents of happiness and pleasure, belonging, and reactive-emotional content received a mean score of 3.26 as the second factor. The Role schema with its subcomponents of socialization and social bonding, participation, and community life received a mean score of 3.06 as the third factor. The Fact and Concept schema with its subcomponents of association and finding meaning, conceptualization, memories and lessons, operational perception and inferential perception received a mean score of 3.02 as the fourth factor. The Strategy/Procedure schema with its subcomponents of attention, cognitive mapping and decision making received a mean score of 2.85 as the fifth factor. Finally, the Self/Person schema with its subcomponents of identity and independence received the lowest mean score (2.65) as the least important factor in the formation of traditional housing pattern in Maragheh. The ultimate achievement of this research indicates that there is continuity among all schemes in traditional housing patterns. The patterns seem to have been continuous and gradual. In contemporary society, however, schemas are undergoing dramatic changes and corresponding to them, changes occur in effective schemas in spatial patterns. Therefore, in contemporary housing design, it is necessary to adjust spatial solutions to allow for the shift from the current patterns of housing to desirable cultural patterns in contemporary housing with a comprehensive understanding of mental schemas at both levels of common and ideal cultural schemas.

Graphical Abstract

The Role of Mental Schemes in the Formation of Traditional Maragheh Housing Pattern

Keywords

Main Subjects


اسمیت، فیلیپ. 1383. درآمدی بر نظریه­های فرهنگی. ترجمه حسن پویان. تهران: دفتر پژوهش­های فرهنگی.
 بهشتی، سید محمد. 1395. خانه (از آغاز پهلوی تا دهه چهل شمسی). مطالعات معماری ایران (10): 242-229.
حائری مازندرانی، محمدرضا. 1388. خانه، فرهنگ، طبیعت. تهران: مرکز مطالعاتی و تحقیقاتی معماری و شهرسازی.
سلطانی، مهرداد و منصوری، سید امیر و فرزین، احمد علی. 1391. تطبیق نقش الگو و مفاهیم مبتنی بر تجربه در فضای معماری. باغ نظر 9 (21): 3-14.
شکوری، رضا و خدادادی، الهام. 1393. طرح‌واره سکونت عشایر نمونه موردی عشایر قشقایی سمیرم. باغ نظر 11(28): 3-14.
لنگ، جان. 1390. آفرینش نظریه معماری. ترجمه علیرضا عینی­فر. تهران: انتشارات دانشگاه تهران.
معماریان، غلامحسین. 1393. مبنای طراحی معماری. تهران: انتشارات نغمه نواندیش.
معماریان، غلامحسین و دهقانی تفتی، محسن. 1396. در جستجوی معنایی نو بر مفهوم گونه و گونه­شناسی در معماری (مطالعه موردی: خانه گونه تالاردار شهر تفت). مسکن محیط و روستا. 37 (162): 21-38.
یزدان­فر، سید عباس و ناصردوست، زهره و ضرابی­الحسینی، مهسا. 1395. الگوی مسکن مراغه با توجه به شیوه زندگی در گذر زمان (از دوره زندیه تا امروز). تهران: موسسه انتشارات دانشگاه تهران.
Arnheim, R. (1969). Visual Thinking, Faber and Faber, London.
Augoustinos, M, Walker, I, Donaghue, N. (1995). Social cognition, an integrated introduction, London, Sage.
Barsalow, L.W, Sewell, D.R. (1985). Contrasting the representation of scripts and categories, Journal of Memory and Language, No 24, pp.  646–665.
Bartlett, F. C. (1954). Remembering, Cambridge University, Cambridge, England.
Brewer, W. F, Treyens, J. C. (1981). Role of Schemata in Memory for Places, Cognitive Psychology, No13, pp. 207-230.
Canter, D. (1991). Understanding, assessing, and acting in places: is an integrative framework possible? In Environment, Cognition, and Action, edited by T. Garling, and G. Evans. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 191- 209.
Cantor, N, Mischel, W. (1979). Prototypes in person perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology ,Vol 12, pp. 3-52.
Chi, M. T. H. (1981). Knowledge development and memory performance. In M. P. Friedman, J. P. Das & N. O’Conner (Eds.), Intelligence and learning, pp. 221–229.
Fiske, S. T, Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social Cognition (2nd ed.), McGrawHill, New York.
Genereux, R. L, Ward, L.M, Russell, J.A. (1995), The Behavioural Component in the Meaning of Places, Journal of Environmental Psychology, Vol 3, No 1, pp. 43-55.
Gibson, James, J. (1966). The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems, Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Gilstrap, L, Livia, L, Laub, C. (2005). Schema Encyclopedia of Human Development. CA: SAGE publications.
Hampton, J. A. (1982). An investigation of the nature of abstract concepts. Memory & Cognition, Vol 9, No 2, pp. 149–156.
Kohls, C, Scheiter, K. (2008). The Relation Between Design Patterns and Schema Theory, in Proceedings of the 15th Pattern Languages of Programs Conference, pp. 18 - 20.
Lindsay, P, Norman, D. A. (1977). Human information processing: an introduction to psychology. Academic Press, NewYork.
Mandler, G. (1982). The Structure of Value: Accounting for Taste, Affect And Cognition: The Seventeenth Annual Carnegie Symposium On Cognition, eds. M. S. Clark and S. T. Fiske, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, New Jersey; pp. 3-36.
Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the self, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol 35, No 2, pp. 63–78.
Neisser, U. (1976). Cogniton and Reality. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman.
Nishida, H. (1999). A cognitive approach to intercultural communication based on schema theory, International Journal of Intercultural Relations, Vol 23, No 5, pp. 753–777.
Onal, G.K. (2010). A Method for Investigation the Effects of Student's Cultural Schema on Architectural Design Process in Architectural Design Education (Unpublished PhD Thesis). Istanbul Technical University, Istanbul, Turkey.
Palmer, G.B. (1996). Toward a Theory of Cultural Linguistics, Austin: University of Texas Press.
Pezzulo,G, Calvi,G. (2007). Designing modular architectures in the framework akira, Multiagent and Grid Systems, Vol 3, No 1, pp. 65-86.
Piaget, J. (1954). The Construction of Reality in the Child. Ballentine.
-Puecell, A. T. (1986). Environmental perception and affect. Environment and behavior , Vol. 18 No. 1, 3-30.
Rapoport, A. (1982). The meaning of the built environment: a nonverbal communication approach, Arizona, University of Arizona Press.
Rapoport, A. (1990). The Meaning of the Built Environment, 2nded, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona.
Rosch, E. H, Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblances: studies in the internal structure of categories, Cognitive Psychology, Vol 7, No 4, pp. 573–605.
Schank, R. C, Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, Plans, Goals, and Understanding, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
Seifert, K. L (1991). Educational Psychology, 2nd ed., Boston: Houghton Mifin.
Shaw, R, Pittenger, J. (1977). Perceiving the face of change in changing faces: implications for a theory of object perception. In R. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.), Perceiving, acting and knowing: toward an ecological psychology , pp. 103–132. Hillsdale, N
Snodgrass, J, Russell, J.A, Ward, L.M. (1988). Planning, Mood, and Place-Liking, Journal of Environmental Psychology, Vol 8, No 3, pp. 209-222.
Stokols, D. (1981). Group X Place Transactions: Some Neglected Issues in Psychological Research on Settings, Toward a Psychology of Situations: An Interactional Perspective, ed. D. Magnusson, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, New Jersey; pp. 393-415.
Strandell, J. (2017). The Cultural Schema: Toward Conceptual Compatibility in Culture-Cognition Interaction Research. In book: Culture-Cognition.
Taylor, S. E, Crocker, J. (1981). Schematic bases of social information processing. In E. T.      Higgins, C. P, Herman, M. P, Zanna (Eds.), Social cogni- 776 H. Nishidation: the Ontario symposium,Vol 1, pp. 89–134.
Turgut, H. (1990). A Method for the determination of culturebehavior-space interaction system (PhD Thesis). Istanbul Technical University, Turkey.
Turner, R. M. (1994). Adaptive reasoning for real-world problems: a schemabased approach, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Ward, L.M, Snodgrass, J, Chew, B, Russell, J.A. (1988). The Role of Plans in Cognitive and Affective Responses to Places, Journal of Environmental Psychology, Vol 8, No 1, pp. 1-8.